Northwestern University

12/17/2024 | Press release | Distributed by Public on 12/17/2024 09:35

Study sheds new light on infants’ ability to learn from nonlinguistic signals

Study sheds new light on infants' ability to learn from nonlinguistic signals

Infants as young as six months can flexibly recruit non-language sounds and bend them to their communicative needs

Media Information

  • Release Date: December 17, 2024

Media Contacts

Stephanie Kulke

EVANSTON, Ill. --- A new study by developmental scientists at Northwestern University provides new insight into a hallmark of human communication: our flexibility to use non-linguistic signals to serve our communication needs.

Just as humans can use the taps of Morse Code or the patterns of smoke signals to communicate precise messages, infants show a remarkable flexibility to interpret nonlinguistic signals to aid their learning.

But what conditions are required for babies to elevate new nonlinguistic signals in this way? And how early can they do so?

Sandra Waxman, the study's senior author, and her colleagues discovered that infants as young as six months old were able to harness nonlinguistic signals for learning, a surprising finding because at this age, babies are just beginning to acquire their own language.

The evidence revealed the conditions under which babies conferred communicative status to the novel tone signals and then recruited them to successfully complete a learning task. Infants' success did not depend on whether the signals were produced by humans, or in a give-and-take interchange between individuals. Instead, what mattered was cross-modal temporal synchrony, in other words, if the method of signal delivery included synchronized sound and movement.

The findings were published recently in Nature Journal's Scientific Reports.

Waxman is the Louis W. Menk professor of psychology and the director of the Infant and Child Development Center at Northwestern. Co-authors are Brock Ferguson, who completed a Ph.D. at Northwestern and worked on the study; and Alexander LaTourrette, who completed a Ph.D. at Northwestern and is now assistant professor of psychology at the University of Southern California.

"The study is significant because it helps us to understand what capacities the human mind is furnished with from the start and how infants use those capacities to identify important sources and signals for learning," Waxman said.

How the study was done

Sine-wave tone signals were chosen as a control in the studies because prior research has shown tones on their own are neutral and can't help babies in categorization or other tasks. They also knew from their previous research, that it is possible to confer communication status to tones.

In this study, six-month-old babies were first introduced to novel tone sequences presented in a short animated video featuring two non-human agents - a triangle and a circle. Next, infants participated in a learning task. If the cues provided in the videos were sufficient to permit infants to infuse the tones with communicative status, then they should recruit tone signals later, using them to advance their performance in the subsequent learning task.

In part one of the study, the video showed a conversation between two agents, one agent producing tone sequences, and the other speaking English sentences. In part two, the English sentences were replaced with tone sequences. In part three, only one of the two agents moved, and did so in perfect temporal synchrony with the tone sequences, as if producing them. In part four, the same agent moved out, but the synchrony between the agent's movement and the tone sequences was disrupted.

The researchers were surprised to find the babies were able to elevate the signals to communication status in all three conditions in which the signal was produced synchronously with the motion of the agent.

"Even in the third video where one agent sat like a bump on a log, and the other bounced and beeped, the exposure still helped assign meaning to the tone," Waxman said.

"This is an important finding: When we provided cross-modal synchrony between the tones and the agents' motions, infants spontaneously elevated tone signals to communicative status. We think that when we disrupted this synchrony between different senses, we were effectively 'telling' the baby that the sounds were not produced by the agent, a potential source of learning."

Implications and next steps

This study showed, for first time, that the remarkable human capacity to elevate signals other than language to communicative status does not develop late and does not require a fully developed linguistic system as a foundation. Instead, this flexibility is available to infants from the beginning of language acquisition.

The study specified with precision what makes nonlinguistic signals communicative. It also opens new avenues for research, including tracing how an infant's own experience with other candidate communicative signals, like pointing and eye-gaze, yield downstream advantages for learning.

"This work offers valuable insights into theories of language acquisition. But equally important, it holds promise for designing effective interventions for infants and young children facing developmental delays and impairments," Waxman said.